Thursday, August 28, 2014

Women in Psychology: Mary Ainsworth

Mary Dinsmore Salter Ainworth, an American psychologist who specialized in the study of infant attachment was born December 1, 1913 in Glendale Ohio.  Mary was the oldest of three sisters and both of her parents were graduates from Dickinson College.  When Mary was five years old her father earned his Master’s in History degree and was transferred to a manufacturing firm in Canada where he moved his family.
William McDougall’s book Character and the Conduct of Life is what inspired Mary Ainsworth interest in psychology at the age of 15.  In this book McDougall endeavours to make practical application of his psychology for the benefit of those who desire to be usefully happy and blamelessly successful.  He hopes that it may be found to go a little farther than others of similar aim in affording practical guidance in the conduct of life, and whatever view we may take of the attempt, it can scarcely be denied that this modest hope is amply justified.  The book falls into two fairly distinct parts—the first dealing with the more formal and general aspects of conduct, the second with the more material and specific aspects ("Character and the Conduct of Life: Practical Psychology for every man," 1927).  Mary went on to attend the University of Toronto and was enrolled in the honors psychology program. 
In an article about the secure child Mary Ainsworth stated that “Throughout my entire career the underlying aim has been the understanding of intimate interpersonal relationships, especially the earliest of these, and they influence subsequent personality development.  Undoubtedly it was the interest –then half-recognized—that led me to choose to study psychology when an undergraduate at the University of Toronto in the early 1930s” (Volpe, 2010). 
Mary describes her primary interest in psychology increased while attending Professor William Blatz’s course in genetic and abnormal psychology in which he introduced the student’s to his “security theory”.   Security theory is essentially a theory about personality development.  It can be characterized as an open-ended theory (Volpe, 2010).  At the same time, and experimental project directed by Professor Sperrin Chant taught Mary that research could be fascinating.  Professor Blatz suggested that Mary begin dissertation research relevant to his security theory under the supervision of him and Professor Chant.  Mary’s dissertation was completed in 1939 and published in the following year (Salter, 1940).  It was believed to be the first publication stemming from Blatzian security theory.   Mary stated that these were the reasons that inspired her to stay at the University of Toronto as a Graduate where she earned her B.A. in 1935, her M.A. in 1936 and her Ph.D. in 1939 (Volpe, 2010) .
After obtaining her Ph.D., from the University of Toronto, Mary began her career as a professor at the University of Toronto before joining the Canadian Women’s Army Corp in 1942 during World War II where she was assigned to the Directorate of Personnel Selection.  After a brief period of post-war government services as the superintendent of Women’s Rehabilitation in the Canadian Department of Veteran’s Affairs, Mary returned to the University of Toronto to teach personality psychology and conduct research in the assessment of security (Arcus, 2001).
In 1950 Mary married Leonard Ainsworth, a graduate student at the University of Toronto.  This marriage created a conflict of interest on Mary’s behalf because she was a faculty member at the university so the couple relocated to England where Leonard accepted a doctoral program at the University College, London.  Soon after that Mary began a research position at the Tavistock Clinic with John Bowlby, who was using evolutionary and ethological theory to explore the development of attachments to caregivers and the consequences of maternal separation and loss for young children (Darity Jr., 2008).  The research team at the Tavistock clinic studied the effects of maternal separation on child development.  Comparison of disrupted mother-child bonds to normal mother-child relationship showed that a child’s lack of a mother figure leads to “adverse development effects.”
Mary’s work at the Tavistock clinic with Bowlby caused her earlier interest in security and developmental surface and she created a plan to conduct a longitudinal study of mother-infant interaction in a natural setting at her earliest opportunity.  That opportunity came when her husband accepted a position in the East African Institute of Social Research in Kampala, Uganda.  It was there in Uganda that Mary studied mothers and infants in their natural environment, observing and recording as much as possible, and analyzing and publishing the data years later after joining the faculty at John Hopkins University of Baltimore (Arcus, 2001).
According to the Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology, Mary concluded that there are qualitatively distinct patterns of attachment that evolve between infants and their mothers over the opening years of life.  This was based on her original observations in Uganda and subsequent studies in Baltimore. This was known as the Baltimore project where she visited the homes of 26 families after a child was born up until they turned twelve months.  Her findings were detailed narratives of mother-infant interactions. Mary stated that although a majority of these patterns are marked by comfort And security, some are tense or conflicted, and she found evidence suggesting that these relationships were related to the level of responsiveness that mothers showed toward their infants from the earliest months.  In one study she found mothers who responded more quickly to their infants cries at three months were more likely to have developed secure attachments with their babies by year one (Arcus, 2001) . 
While teaching at John Hopkins University Mary and her colleagues began working on creating an assessment to measure attachments between mothers and children.  It was at John Hopkins University the she developed her famous ‘Strange Situation” assessment, which is said to be one of the most common used procedures in child development research (Arcus, 2001).  The Strange Situation has also been extended to the studies of attachment behaviors and correlates in rhesus monkeys, chimpanzees, and dogs used as pets and guide animals for the blind (Darity Jr., 2008) . 
The Strange Situation is a twenty-minute procedure where there is a series of separations and reunions between mother and toddler.  Three main patterns of attachments were observed: (1) anxious/avoidant, in which the child tended not to be distressed at the mother’s departure and to avoid her on return; (2) securely attached, in which the child was distressed by mother’s departure and easily soothed by her on return; and (3) anxious/resistant, in which the child tended to become highly distressed at the mother’s departure, only to seek comfort and distance simultaneously on her return by engaging in behaviors as crying and reaching to be held, but then attempting to leave once picked up (Darity Jr., 2008) . A fourth category disorganized/disoriented attachment was added by one of Mary Ainsworth colleagues, Mary Main in which a child may cry during separation but avoid the mother when she returns or may approach the mother, then freeze or fall to the floor.  Some show stereotyped behavior, rocking to and fro or repeatedly hitting themselves.  Mary Main found that most of the mothers of these children had suffered major losses or other trauma shortly before or after birth of the infant and had reacted by becoming severely depressed.  In fact, 56% of mothers who had lost a parent by death before they completed high school subsequently was said to have children with disorganized attachments (Solomon, 1990).
Mary Ainsworth’s research and development of the “Strange Situation” assessment has played an important role in the understanding of child development.  She contributes her desire to move forward with the research to William Blatz’s “security theory” because she believed that it was the concept of security itself that was a guiding principle for her work on showing how early attachment styles contribute to later behaviors (Volpe, 2010).


No comments:

Post a Comment